Contributions of Neuroscience Thesis
Contributions of Neuroscience Thesis
Contributions of Neuroscience to Our Understanding of Cognitive Development Adele Diamond1 and Dima Amso2
1 Department of Psychiatry, University of British Columbia, and Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry,
BC Children’s Hospital, Vancouver, Canada; and 2 Sackler Institute for Developmental Psychobiology,
Weill Medical College of Cornell University
ABSTRACT—One major contribution of neuroscience to
understanding cognitive development has been in demon-
strating that biology is not destiny—that is, demonstrating
the remarkable role of experience in shaping the mind,
brain, and body. Only rarely has neuroscience provided
wholly new insights into cognitive development, but often
it has provided evidence of mechanisms by which obser-
vations of developmental psychologists could be explained.
Behavioral findings have often remained controversial
until an underlying biological mechanism for them was
offered. Neuroscience has demonstrated promise for de-
tecting cognitive problems before they are behaviorally
observable—and, hence, promise for early intervention. In
this article, we discuss examples drawn from imitation and
mirror neurons, phenylketonuria (PKU) and prefrontal
dopamine, maternal touch and stress reactivity, and non-
genetic (behavioral) intergenerational transmission of bi-
ological characteristics.
KEYWORDS—plasticity; epigenesis; mothering; executive
functions; animal models; molecular genetics; memory
Neuroscience research has made its greatest contributions to the
study of cognitive development by illuminating mechanisms
(providing a ‘‘how’’) that underlie behavioral observations made
earlier by psychologists. It has also made important contribu-
tions to our understanding of cognitive development by dem-
onstrating that the brain is far more plastic at all ages than
previously thought—and thus that the speed and extent by which
experience and behavior can shape the brain is greater than al-
most anyone imagined. In other words, rather than showing that
biology is destiny, neuroscience research has been at the fore-
front of demonstrating the powerful role of experience throughout
life. Besides the surprising evidence of the remarkable extent
of experience-induced plasticity, rarely has neuroscience given
us previously unknown insights into cognitive development, but
neuroscience does offer promise of being able to detect some
problems before they are behaviorally observable.
PROVIDING MECHANISMS THAT CAN ACCOUNT FOR
BEHAVIORAL RESULTS REPORTED BY
PSYCHOLOGISTS
Here we describe two examples of behavioral findings by psy-
chologists that were largely ignored or extremely controversial
until underlying biological mechanisms capable of accounting
for them were provided by neuroscience research. One such
example concerns cognitive deficits documented in children
treated early and continuously for phenylketonuria (PKU). The
second example involves neonatal imitation observed by psy-
chologists and mirror neurons discovered by neuroscientists.
Prefrontal Dopamine System and PKU Cognitive Deficits
Since at least the mid-1980s, psychologists were reporting
cognitive deficits in children with PKU that resembled those
associated with frontal cortex dysfunction (e.g., Pennington,
VanDoornick, McCabe, & McCabe, 1985). Those reports did not
impact medical care, however. Doctors were skeptical. No one
could imagine a mechanism capable of producing what psy-
chologists claimed to be observing.
PKU is a disorder in the gene that codes for phenylalanine
hydroxylase, an enzyme essential for the conversion of phenyl-
alanine (Phe) to tyrosine (Tyr). In those with PKU, that enzyme is
Address correspondence to Adele Diamond, Canada Research Chair Professor of Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, Department of Psychiatry, University of British Columbia, 2255 Wesbrook Mall, Vancouver, British Columbia, V6T 2A1, Canada; e-mail: adele. diamond@ubc.ca.
C U R R E N T D I R E C T I O N S I N P S Y C H O L O G I C A L S C I E N C E
136 Volume 17—Number 2Copyright r 2008 Association for Psychological Science
absent or inactive. Without treatment, Phe levels skyrocket,
resulting in gross brain damage and mental retardation. Phe is an
amino acid and a component of all dietary protein. PKU treat-
ment consists primarily of reducing dietary intake of protein to
keep Phe levels down, but that has to be balanced against the
need for protein. For years, children with PKU were considered
adequately treated if their blood Phe levels were below 600
micromoles per liter (mmol/L; normal levels in the general public being 60–120 mmol/L). Such children did not have mental re- tardation and showed no gross brain damage, although no one
disputed that their blood Phe levels were somewhat elevated and
their blood Tyr levels were somewhat reduced (Tyr levels were
not grossly reduced because even though the hydroxylation of
Phe into Tyr was largely inoperative, Tyr is also available in
protein). Since Phe and Tyr compete to cross into the brain, a
modest increase in the ratio of Phe to Tyr in the bloodstream
results in a modest decrease in how much Tyr can reach the
brain. Note that this is a global effect—the entire brain receives
somewhat too little Tyr. How was it possible to make sense of
psychologists’ claims that the resulting cognitive deficits were
not global but limited to the cognitive functions dependent on
prefrontal cortex?